Welcome to International Journal of Research in Social Sciences & HumanitiesE-ISSN : 2249 - 4642 | P-ISSN: 2454 - 4671 IMPACT FACTOR: 8.561 |
Abstract
IMPACT OF PARENTAL DISCIPLINE METHODS ON THE CHILD'S INTERNALIZATION OF VALUES
Achla Tyagi
Volume: 1 Issue: 1 2011
Abstract:
The goals of the present study were: 1) to describe and provide initial support for the validity of the Future Scenarios Questionnaire (FSQ), a new self-report questionnaire designed to measure parental responding to anticipated children’s negative emotions; and 2) to examine how maternal responses on the FSQ related to young children’s aggressive, asocial, and prosocial behaviours with peers. Further, this study examined whether the temperamental trait of negative affect moderated the relation between maternal responses on the FSQ and children’s social adjustment outcomes. In particular, encouraging emotion expression significantly predicted more asocial behavior and less prosocial behavior (approached significance), but only for children rated high in negative affect. Similarly, discouraging emotion expression significantly predicted less aggressive behavior only for high negative affect children. None of these Relations was significant for children rated low in negative affect. The theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed in terms of the importance of considering child temperament in emotion socialization processes
References
- Aber, J. L., Slade, A., Berger, B., Bresgi, I., & Kaplan, M. (1985).The Parent Development Interview. Unpublished manuscript.
- Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2006). Development of displaced speech in early mother- child conversations. Child Development, 77, 186-200.
- Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
- Altshuler, J. L., & Ruble, D. N. (1989). Developmental changes inchildren's awareness of strategies for coping with uncontrollable stress. Child evelopment, 60, 1337-1349.
- Arcus, D. (2001). Inhibited and uninhibited children: Biology in the social context. In T. D.
- Wachs & G. A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Temperament in context (pp.43-60). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Bates, J. E. & Pettit, G. S. (2007). Temperament, parenting, and socialization. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 153-177). New York: Guilford.
- Bell, R. Q., & Chapman, M. (1986). Child effects in studies using experimental or brief longitudinal approaches to socialization.Developmental Psychology, 22, 595-603.
- Belsky, J. (2005). Differential susceptibility to rearing influence: An evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In B. J. Ellis & D.
- F. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 139-163). New York: Guilford.
- Belsky, J., Hsieh, K., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys' externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Development and Psychopathology, 10,301-319.
- Benson, J. B. (1997). The development of planning: It's about time. In S. L. Friedman & E. K. Scholnick (Eds.), The developmental psychology of planning: Why, how, and when do we plan? (pp. 43-75). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Bugental, D. B., Blue, J., & Lewis, J. (1990). Caregiver beliefs and dysphoric affect directed to difficult children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 631-638.
- Bugental, D. B., & Happaney, K. (2002). Parental attributions. In M.H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent (2nd ed.) (pp. 509-535). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Bugental, D. B., Johnston, C., New, M., & Silvester, J. (1998). Measuring parental attributions: Conceptual and methodological issues. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 459-480.
- Bugental, D. B., & Shennum, W. A. (1984). 'Difficult' children aselicitors and targets of adult communication patterns: An attributional-behavioral transactional analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49.
- Creasey, G., Mitts, N., & Catanzaro, S. (1995). Associations among daily hassles, coping, and behavior problems in nonreferred kindergartners. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24, 311-319.
- De Gucht, V., Fontaine, J., & Fischler, B. (2004). Temporalstability and differential relationships with neuroticism andextraversion of the three subscales of the 20-item TorontoAlexithymia Scale in clinical and nonclinical samples. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 57, 25-33.
- DeOliveira, C. A., Moran, G., & Pederson, D. R. (2005). Understanding the link between maternal adult attachment classifications and thoughts and feelings about emotions.
- Attachment & Human Development, 7, 153-170.
- Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194-201.
- Denham, S. A. (1993). Maternal emotional responsiveness andtoddlers' socialemotional competence. Journal of ChildPsychology and Psychiatry, 34, 715-728.
- Denham, S. A. (1997). 'When I have a bad dream mommy holds me': Preschoolers' conceptions of emotions, parental socialisation, and emotional competence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 301-319.
- Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. New York: Guilford.
- Denham, S. A., & Auerbach, S. (1995). Mother-child dialogue about emotions and preschoolers emotional competence Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 121, 313- 337.
Refer & Earn |